By cooperating with what they think is the first speaker's
intent, the respondents place themselves unknowingly in a vulnerable posi-
tion.
intent, the respondents place themselves unknowingly in a vulnerable posi-
tion.
Childens - Folklore
handle.
net/2027/usu.
39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www.
hathitrust.
org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3.
0
? ? 28.
Other answers, however, would fit just as easily: "a baby crying," "some-
one inside a well looking up at the moon at night," or "someone inside a
tunnel looking back at the entrance. " Nothing about the sketch itself nec-
essarily makes one of these explanations more likely than any of the others.
Assuming that it can be made to fit the graphic data, the correct answer is
quite simply the one that the riddler says is correct.
Few visual descriptive riddles have been collected from American
schoolchildren. There are indications, however, that this genre forms an ac-
tive part of youngsters' riddle repertoires. Mary and Herbert Knapp (1976,
229) report that children sometimes exchange visual riddles in the classroom
when the teacher's back is turned. In addition, college students (Roemer
1982a) have reported that they knew and told such riddles as children, ex-
changing them, for instance, on Scout bus trips.
At present, most of the published material on the genre has come from
the European youngsters. In her study of Finnish "Children's Lore" (1978,
56-58), Leea Virtanen gives twelve visual riddles. To our loss, however, she
provides the riddles without discussion:
29. FIG. Grandmother can't swim (Virtanen 1978, 57).
179
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? To date, the most extensive collection has been offered by Bengt af Klintberg
(1980). Klintberg gained most of his material (seventy-five riddles) through
questionnaires sent to sixth-grade students (twelve years old) throughout
Sweden. The material returned showed "only insignificant" regional varia-
tions, suggesting that the tradition is relatively uniform across the country.
Among Swedish schoolchildren, Klintberg reports, the riddles are most fre-
quently termed bildgdtor ("picture riddles"). 1
The major strength of Klintberg's report is its discussion of histori-
cal and comparative issues. Klintberg traces much of the popularity of vi-
sual riddles in Sweden to American influence and particularly to the books
of the American humorist Roger Price. A few of the riddles imported to
Sweden have developed interesting quirks as the result of the language dif-
ferences. Among American informants, Figure 30 is often explained as "a
navel orange wearing a bikini. " Because of translation problems, however,
none of the Swedish riddles using the analogous Figure 31 have retained the
"navel orange" answer. Instead, the Swedish orange is drawn without a "na-
vel," and the usual answer is "orange in a bikini" (1980, 196):
30. FIG. A navel orange 31. FIG. Orange in a bikini
wearing a bikini (Klintberg 1980, 193).
(Roemer, unpublished data).
"
Although Klintberg's report provides very useful information, we as
yet have no details on children's development of interest and competence in
the use of the genre. Nor do we know if certain visual riddles are more ap-
propriate among certain age groups. In addition, we have little information
as to how the riddles are exchanged. Among American college students, vi-
sual riddles and rebuses are exchanged during the same interactional sessions
18o RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? (Roemer 1981-82 unpublished data; Preston 1982). Does this hold true for
sessions among schoolchildren? Similar points need to be raised concerning
the other nonverbal genres surveyed in this section.
RHETORICAL IMPACT
True and Joking Riddles
Conventionally, the riddles treated in this chapter have been separated into
categories depending on their solvability. Generally speaking, folklorists have
termed riddles "true" if their answers could be reasoned out, based on in-
formation supplied in the riddle proposition and the respondent's adequate
experience with recall of tropes, symbols, and other conventions shared
within the particular culture.
What have been called "true riddles" are based primarily in descrip-
tion (for example, nos. 1, 2, 4, and 8) and have attracted considerable at-
tention in the literature (Taylor 1951). In addition, they enjoy a measured
popularity among American and British youngsters. Though some of the
longer or more poetic forms (for example, nos. 4-8) remain current, urban
children seem more attracted to riddles that are concise and brief. Mary and
Herbert Knapp (1976, 109) cite children's tendency to abridge the longer
forms (no. 32), creating brief and especially enigmatic descriptions (no. 33):
32. In marble walls as white as milk,
Lined with skin as soft as silk,
Within a crystal fountain clear,
A golden apple doth appear.
No doors there are to this stronghold,
Yet thieves break in to steal the gold. -an egg.
33. No doors there are to this stronghold
But thieves break in and take the gold. -an egg.
In addition, there has been a shift from statementlike descriptions to those
using an explicit interrogative. Compared with the versions of the "egg" riddle
above, no. 34 evidences both severe abridgement and interrogative form:
34. What house has no door? -an egg. (McCosh 1979, 165)11
The features of brevity and interrogative form are apparent in most of the
true riddles found in urban children's repertoires. Indeed, one of the most
I8i
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? popular of these demonstrates these characteristics:
35. What's black and white and red all over? -a newspaper.
The ubiquity of the "newspaper riddle" (Barrick 1974) has fostered a range
of alternate answers: "a blushing zebra," "a skunk with diaper rash," "a
bleeding nun," "an integration march," and "an Afro-American Santa
Claus" (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 108; see also Weiner 1970, 37-38; McCosh
1976, 176-77; and Bronner 1988, 288-90 ff. ). With these answers, the hom-
onymic play in the question disappears and "red" is interpreted merely as
the name of a color. When coupled with an answer such as "a blushing ze-
bra," the interrogative shifts from the status of a true riddle question to that
of a question in a joking riddle (discussed below).
Routines that interfere markedly in their own interpretation can be
divided into two subgroups. Visual descriptive riddles and gestural riddles
inhibit deciphering by the very flexibility of their codes. Outside of the or-
thographic code, which visual descriptive riddles do not address anyway,
there exists no standard "grammar" for interpreting individual squiggles,
dots, and blotches inscribed on a page. A line may mean one thing in one
drawing and quite another in a second drawing. Furthermore, because it is
the riddle answer that gives significance to the inscription, a riddle with a
variety of answers can interpret its drawing in a variety of ways (for example,
no. 28). Similar points can be raised with gestural riddles, but there the re-
spondent must work not only with basically uncoded data but also with data
that are kinesic and therefore highly ephemeral.
Joking riddles are the second category of forms that are extremely
difficult of solution. To be sure, joking routines can differ in the quality of
their humor, but, generally speaking, the propositions serve primarily as a
setup for the punch line of the answer:
36. What is the worst weather for rats and mice? --when it's rain-
ing cats and dogs. (Leventhal and Cray 1963, 254)
37. What's tall and says eef eif [eof] muf? -a backward giant.
(McCosh 1976, 176)
THE PARODIC IMPULSE
Parodic forms are not so much a distinct category of riddle as they are forms
that extend the humor of joking riddles into absurdity and nonsense. Pa-
rodic riddles adopt the organizational strategies of more conservative forms.
i8z RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? What marks them as extensions is the degree of violence they do to assump-
tions of everyday life.
Parodic riddles are very popular among urban children, and various
researchers have commented on this fascination. Alto Jablow and Carl With-
ers (1965) view the riddles as safety-valve mechanisms that enable young-
sters to express their amused, angry, or frightened awareness of the fast-
paced, violent, and often irrational world in which they live. If the routines
are absurd and meaningless, Jablow and Withers argue, it is because urban
society itself reflects those characteristics (1965, 252-55). For their part,
Mary and Herbert Knapp (1976) agree that children may become connois-
seurs of chaos in buffering themselves against modern life. The Knapps,
however, also advance an alternate explanation. Urban children, they sug-
gest, have developed sophisticated tastes in their folk humor. Because pa-
rodic forms deal in ambiguity and nonsense, they speak to children's almost
insatiable fascination with complex and startling relationships (1976, 111).
Some parodic riddles can be used to challenge assumptions about
what is possible in the everyday world. We can understand how an answer
fits its question, yet what the riddle as a whole proposes is nonsensical given
a conventional understanding of the "real" world. If what these riddles sug-
gest were valid, we would need to reorganize our conception of the world
around us. Examples of parodic forms include the following:12
Description:
38. What's big and grey with a purple spot in the middle? -an
elephant hit by a Comanche grape. (Weiner 1970, 32)
39. What's green and flies from planet to planet? -an interplan-
etary cucumber. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 110)
40. (Snap your fingers in the air several times, jerking your hand
up and down. ) "What's this? "-a butterfly with hiccups (or, a
drunk butterfly). (Leventhal and Cray 1963, 249)
41. FIG. A clam with buck teeth (Roemer, unpublished data).
183
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? Contrast:
42. What's the difference between unloading dead babies and un-
loading bowling balls? -you can't use a pitchfork to unload bowl-
ing balls. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 111)
Classification/Definition:
43. What do you call a werewolf in a Dacron suit? --a wash and
werewolf. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 110)
Effect/Cause:
44. Why do elephants have flat feet? -from jumping out of palm
trees. (McCosh 1976, 224)
Catch Riddles
Using metacommunicative techniques, other riddles violate the conventions
of riddling as a particular type of social exchange. No. 45 below challenges
the expectation that a riddler's question will provide information relevant
to the answer. Here, the respondents receive only part of the formulaic in-
formation characteristic of contrast riddles. Beyond that, the respondents are
on their own:
45. A: What's the difference?
B: Between what?
A: On this one I'm giving you no clues. (Jablow and Withers
1965, 249)
Other riddles are iconoclastic in other ways. No. 46 begins with a descrip-
tive question, the conventional way of initiating some of the most conser-
vative of English-language riddles. It concludes, however, by challenging the
assumption that riddlers know the answer to the questions they pose:
46. What's red, purple, green, yellow, gray, purple, sky-blue, and
green?
-I don't know, that's why I'm asking you. (McCosh 1976, 178)
A related surprise awaits respondents with no. 47. In addition to not sup-
184 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? plying a satisfactory resolution, the riddle threatens the respondent's assump-
tion of physical security:
47. A: What has six legs, fuzzy ears, and a long tail?
B: What?
A: I don't know, but there's one on your back. (McCosh 1976,
178)
In this and other catch riddles (Roemer 1977), the respondents are set up
for victimization.
By cooperating with what they think is the first speaker's
intent, the respondents place themselves unknowingly in a vulnerable posi-
tion. The trickster capitalizes on this vulnerability at the riddle's conclusion:
Description:
48. What word starts with F and ends with CK? -firetruck.
(Winslow 1966c, 172)
Contrast:
49. A: What's the difference between an egg and an elephant?
B: I don't know, what?
A: If you don't know, I'll never send you to get eggs from the
shop. (McCosh 1976, 178)
Cause-Effect
50. If you threw a white ball into a black sea, what would it be-
come? -wet. (McCosh 1976, 181)
THE ACQUISITION OF RIDDLE COMPETENCE
Though research is still in its early stages, we have found that youngsters
do not learn to riddle simply by memorizing and repeating set pieces. In-
stead, their competence proceeds through several stages and includes knowl-
edge of riddle production as well as of riddle interaction. Below, I review
this acquisition process and, reflecting the bias in the literature, focus on
urban American children.
MADE-UP ROUTINES
Youngsters acquire much of their knowledge of riddling in actual riddle in-
185
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? teraction. Some exchanges may be conducted with adults; most often, they
occur with peers. In American society, the peer group represents the single
most important laboratory for youngsters' acquisition and exercise of rid-
dling competence. 14
When exposed to older children's riddling, young children (of around
five years) notice question-answer sequences that call special attention to the
asker. Although they find this sort of performance appealing, young children
usually possess no ready stock of riddles to draw on. Wanting to participate,
they make up their own, trying to imitate those they have heard. As I've
shown above, riddles are based in common solicitational strategies. Young
children depend on these strategies in making up their routines. Among
McDowell's informants (1979, 59-68), the most frequently adopted strate-
gies were those of cause and effect and description:
Causality:
51. How come the pig likes to get in the mud ? -'cause he likes to
take a bath in the mud. (McDowell 1979, 245)
Description:
52. What's red and white, and doesn't do nothing, and has a stick
down its side, and the red and white thing is against the stick? -a
flag. (McDowell 1979, 245)
By far, descriptive routines (like that in no. 52) appear to outnumber those
based in other strategies (Weiner 1970; McDowell 1979).
Made-up descriptive routines enumerate the features or actions of
some object. " Often, the referenced object or some reproduction of it is
within the riddler's immediate environment. Like made-up routines based
on other strategies, those of description do not contain a block element. In-
stead, the ideal routine-according to peer group standards (McDowell
1979)-is accurate and transparent, its answer easy to grasp. The
respondent's correct answer signals that a "good" question has been asked:
53. What's big and has black stripes and white stripes? -a zebra.
(U. T. Children's Folklore Project, unpublished data)16
54. What's real big and it grows in the ground and it's got leaves
on it? -a tree. (U. T. Children's Folklore Project, unpublished data)
186 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? There is some evidence of cultural and regional differences in children's
made-up descriptive routines. With respect to cultural differences, we can
compare examples 53 and 54 above with those below. The former were
obtained from Austin Anglo youngsters; the following, from Austin Mexi-
can American children:
55. It's a little circle in your stomach. -belly button. (McDowell
1979, 244)
56. It's in a hole, what do you call it, in the zoo? -a guinea pig.
(McDowell 1979, 244)
Though children of both groups used solicitations phrased as interrogatives,
only the Mexican American youngsters in addition employed declarative
solicitations (nos. 55, 56 above). This suggests that children from different
cultural backgrounds may exploit linguistic resources differently in their
riddle-making. These differences are suggested when we contrast material
from the Austin youngsters (both Anglo and Mexican American) with ma-
terial obtained in Massachusetts by Meryl Weiner (1970). Like the Austin
children, the Massachusetts youngsters most often relied on the interroga-
tive format. The youngest children in Weiner's sample, however, occasion-
ally framed their descriptions as first-person statements. The riddler pretends
to adopt the identity of the person, place, or thing being described:'7
57. I have a tail. I have a body. I have a face. I am white and
black. What am I?
-a skunk. (Weiner 1970, 9)
This technique does not appear in the Austin material.
THE ACQUISITION OF RIDDLING COMPETENCE
The acquisition of riddling skills is essentially the acquisition of an artistic
competence. Children just entering elementary school have mastered the
basic linguistic resources of their native tongue. They have a rudimentary
grasp of the cognitive systems in their culture. And, through their sponta-
neous play, they have gained practice with the "performance persona"
(McDowell 1979, 187). In learning how to riddle, children learn to apply
these acquired and developing competencies to the specific purposes of riddle
interaction.
Although a number of developmental schemes are available (for ex-
187
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? ample, Weiner 1970, Sanches and Kirshenblatt-Gimblett 1976, Sutton-
Smith 1976b; Wolfenstein 1954), that proposed by McDowell (1979) and
elaborated by Bauman (1977b) best suits our purposes here. In contrast
to the others, this perspective treats riddle acquisition as a social interac-
tional process.
During the initial stage in the acquisition process (around five years),
youngsters learn the basics of riddle interaction. Through observing and
participating, they learn that the poser of a riddle gains the floor, that the
riddle contains both a question and an answer, that the respondent should
be given a chance to guess, and that the poser should know the answer to
his own question (Bauman 1977b, 26). Guided by these observations and
stimulated by the riddles they hear from others, these youngsters make up
their own routines (as discussed below); they also learn that the respondent's
correct answer reflects positively on the riddler. By learning to present such
"successful" routines, young children increase their understanding of the
relationship between riddling and ego-enhancement.
At the second stage (around six years), children tend to modify their
views as to what constitutes a "good" riddle. Now they come to believe that
a riddle is a puzzling question with an arbitrary answer (Bauman 1977b,
27; Sutton-Smith 1976b, 115). Noticing that riddlers always seem to be right,
regardless of the esoteric sequences they come up with, children at this stage
are apt to initiate sequences like the following:
58. riddler: What color is blood?
respondent: Red
riddler: Nope, it's blue and black. (Bauman 1977b, 27)
"Potent elicitations" (Bauman 1977b, 27) such as this don't receive much
peer group reinforcement. Since the relationship between the question and
its answer is grasped only by the riddler (if indeed that), coparticipants aren't
motivated to encourage similar routines. Faced with losing respondents,
young riddlers once again modify their understanding of riddles. Though they
still regard riddles as confusing, children come to assume that riddles must
nevertheless be entertaining. As a result, they turn to the subjects of sexual-
ity and scatology. If they weren't aware of it before, they soon realize that
"dirt" sells:
59. riddler: What do you call people?
respondent: I don't know, what?
riddler: Doo-doo people. (Bauman 1977b, 27)
188 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? Toward the end of the second stage, children's interest in traditional riddles
begins to surface. This burgeoning recognition, however, outstrips the young-
sters' understanding of riddles' speech play. The children can't yet grasp all
of the linguistic and sociolinguistic complexities in the riddles they try to
repeat. As a result, some of their attempts are flawed:
60. What did the mean frog say to the nice frog? -"I hope you
crick" (instead of "croak"). (Bauman 1977b, 28)
As children enter and proceed through the third stage (of around seven
years), their production errors decrease markedly. Youngsters come not only
to recognize the riddle as a traditional form, they are also beginning to ap-
preciate it as such. As this shift is realized, the children increase their efforts
to learn and to produce riddles as preset pieces, with a stable question and
a fixed answer. The display of competence becomes a matter of demonstrat-
ing one's knowledge of riddles within the peer group. '8
In the final stage (at around eight years), children routinely succeed in
the use of traditional riddles. In effect, they have gained mastery of peer-group
riddling. The literature (Wolfenstein 1954; McDowell 1979) suggests that
children maintain an interest in riddling until about the age of ten or twelve.
After that time, they put their performance energies into other genres, allow-
ing their interest in riddling to decline. Since most mainstream American so-
ciety tends to regard riddling as a children's activity and thus as one inappro-
priate for adults except under special circumstances (Roberts and Forman
1972, 182), we might say that youngsters between eight and twelve have prob-
ably acquired as much competence in riddling as they will ever have.
Functions of Children's Riddling
Outside of the fact that it allows its participants to engage in social interac-
tion, there is perhaps no single, universal function of riddling. At the least,
riddle functions result from an interplay among the participants' perception
of situational circumstances, their combined and individual goals, and their
relative commitment to group and community standards. Documenting
riddle functions depends on in-depth fieldwork and the analysis of riddles
in individual communities, groups, and situations.
Below, I review some of the possible riddle functions based on the
factor of social interaction.
Several researchers (for example, Roberts and Forman 1971; Sutton-
Smith 1976b; Bauman 1982) have observed that in many urban groups rid-
dling is a way of engaging in, representing, and commenting on the processes
189
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? of questioning and answering. '9 Children have an interest in playing out and
playing with interrogative formats. For example, riddling models the kind
of interrogation that is ubiquitous in educational settings (Bauman 1982,
184). As a result, children's riddling may in part be an adaptive mechanism,
allowing youngsters to come to terms with the participant structures of
schooling. This perspective has several points that support it. Adult-child
interrogation is characteristic not only of classroom interaction but also of
interaction in the home environment. Parents routinely query children about
the children's activities. It is not surprising, then, that children are attracted
to an activity that allows them to serve in the role of interrogator. That role
allows children some access to power. The children also become the focus
of attention. And, in supplying the riddle answer, they enjoy "being right. "
As an expressive model of interrogation, riddling also permits young-
sters to manipulate the resources of communication. As I've shown, riddles
are based on common solicitational strategies. As framing devices, these strat-
egies serve as tools by which people orient and indicate their perceptions of
the world. In riddling, such strategies facilitate the exchange of information,
for example, by advising respondents of some of the cognitive and commu-
nicative work they are to do. Descriptive questions require that respondents
notice details and generalize from them. Respondents to classification ques-
tions must indicate the categorization of experience. Comparison-contrast
questions require information about similarities or differences in domains
of experience. To be sure, riddles counterpoint these familiar strategies with
a variety of disruptive techniques. True riddles have their block elements;
joking riddles surprise with humor, and parodic forms startle with nonsense
or victimization. These techniques complicate communication, whether the
respondent's task is to solve the riddler's question or to relate its answer to
assumptions of everyday life . The tension, however, is not irresoluble. The
enigma of a true riddle can be solved, the humor of a joking riddle can be
appreciated, and (except for the most drastic of the victimization forms) the
absurdity of a parodic riddle can be dismissed as inconsequential to every-
day concerns.
In concluding, I should point out that children are not likely to be
self-consciously aware of the functions summarized above. For them, rid-
dling is primarily a form of folk entertainment, one that also attests to the
participants' competence in peer group and community traditions.
NOTES TO CHAPTER EIGHT
1. Catch routines, soliciting either a verbal or a physical response, are treated
190 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? in Roemer 1977. The interactional organization and developmental acquisition of
knock-knock routines are discussed in Bauman 1977b. For an extensive collection of
knock-knock routines, see McCosh 1976, 204-12.
2. The riddle proposition is the "question" unit of a riddle.
? ? 28.
Other answers, however, would fit just as easily: "a baby crying," "some-
one inside a well looking up at the moon at night," or "someone inside a
tunnel looking back at the entrance. " Nothing about the sketch itself nec-
essarily makes one of these explanations more likely than any of the others.
Assuming that it can be made to fit the graphic data, the correct answer is
quite simply the one that the riddler says is correct.
Few visual descriptive riddles have been collected from American
schoolchildren. There are indications, however, that this genre forms an ac-
tive part of youngsters' riddle repertoires. Mary and Herbert Knapp (1976,
229) report that children sometimes exchange visual riddles in the classroom
when the teacher's back is turned. In addition, college students (Roemer
1982a) have reported that they knew and told such riddles as children, ex-
changing them, for instance, on Scout bus trips.
At present, most of the published material on the genre has come from
the European youngsters. In her study of Finnish "Children's Lore" (1978,
56-58), Leea Virtanen gives twelve visual riddles. To our loss, however, she
provides the riddles without discussion:
29. FIG. Grandmother can't swim (Virtanen 1978, 57).
179
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? To date, the most extensive collection has been offered by Bengt af Klintberg
(1980). Klintberg gained most of his material (seventy-five riddles) through
questionnaires sent to sixth-grade students (twelve years old) throughout
Sweden. The material returned showed "only insignificant" regional varia-
tions, suggesting that the tradition is relatively uniform across the country.
Among Swedish schoolchildren, Klintberg reports, the riddles are most fre-
quently termed bildgdtor ("picture riddles"). 1
The major strength of Klintberg's report is its discussion of histori-
cal and comparative issues. Klintberg traces much of the popularity of vi-
sual riddles in Sweden to American influence and particularly to the books
of the American humorist Roger Price. A few of the riddles imported to
Sweden have developed interesting quirks as the result of the language dif-
ferences. Among American informants, Figure 30 is often explained as "a
navel orange wearing a bikini. " Because of translation problems, however,
none of the Swedish riddles using the analogous Figure 31 have retained the
"navel orange" answer. Instead, the Swedish orange is drawn without a "na-
vel," and the usual answer is "orange in a bikini" (1980, 196):
30. FIG. A navel orange 31. FIG. Orange in a bikini
wearing a bikini (Klintberg 1980, 193).
(Roemer, unpublished data).
"
Although Klintberg's report provides very useful information, we as
yet have no details on children's development of interest and competence in
the use of the genre. Nor do we know if certain visual riddles are more ap-
propriate among certain age groups. In addition, we have little information
as to how the riddles are exchanged. Among American college students, vi-
sual riddles and rebuses are exchanged during the same interactional sessions
18o RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? (Roemer 1981-82 unpublished data; Preston 1982). Does this hold true for
sessions among schoolchildren? Similar points need to be raised concerning
the other nonverbal genres surveyed in this section.
RHETORICAL IMPACT
True and Joking Riddles
Conventionally, the riddles treated in this chapter have been separated into
categories depending on their solvability. Generally speaking, folklorists have
termed riddles "true" if their answers could be reasoned out, based on in-
formation supplied in the riddle proposition and the respondent's adequate
experience with recall of tropes, symbols, and other conventions shared
within the particular culture.
What have been called "true riddles" are based primarily in descrip-
tion (for example, nos. 1, 2, 4, and 8) and have attracted considerable at-
tention in the literature (Taylor 1951). In addition, they enjoy a measured
popularity among American and British youngsters. Though some of the
longer or more poetic forms (for example, nos. 4-8) remain current, urban
children seem more attracted to riddles that are concise and brief. Mary and
Herbert Knapp (1976, 109) cite children's tendency to abridge the longer
forms (no. 32), creating brief and especially enigmatic descriptions (no. 33):
32. In marble walls as white as milk,
Lined with skin as soft as silk,
Within a crystal fountain clear,
A golden apple doth appear.
No doors there are to this stronghold,
Yet thieves break in to steal the gold. -an egg.
33. No doors there are to this stronghold
But thieves break in and take the gold. -an egg.
In addition, there has been a shift from statementlike descriptions to those
using an explicit interrogative. Compared with the versions of the "egg" riddle
above, no. 34 evidences both severe abridgement and interrogative form:
34. What house has no door? -an egg. (McCosh 1979, 165)11
The features of brevity and interrogative form are apparent in most of the
true riddles found in urban children's repertoires. Indeed, one of the most
I8i
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? popular of these demonstrates these characteristics:
35. What's black and white and red all over? -a newspaper.
The ubiquity of the "newspaper riddle" (Barrick 1974) has fostered a range
of alternate answers: "a blushing zebra," "a skunk with diaper rash," "a
bleeding nun," "an integration march," and "an Afro-American Santa
Claus" (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 108; see also Weiner 1970, 37-38; McCosh
1976, 176-77; and Bronner 1988, 288-90 ff. ). With these answers, the hom-
onymic play in the question disappears and "red" is interpreted merely as
the name of a color. When coupled with an answer such as "a blushing ze-
bra," the interrogative shifts from the status of a true riddle question to that
of a question in a joking riddle (discussed below).
Routines that interfere markedly in their own interpretation can be
divided into two subgroups. Visual descriptive riddles and gestural riddles
inhibit deciphering by the very flexibility of their codes. Outside of the or-
thographic code, which visual descriptive riddles do not address anyway,
there exists no standard "grammar" for interpreting individual squiggles,
dots, and blotches inscribed on a page. A line may mean one thing in one
drawing and quite another in a second drawing. Furthermore, because it is
the riddle answer that gives significance to the inscription, a riddle with a
variety of answers can interpret its drawing in a variety of ways (for example,
no. 28). Similar points can be raised with gestural riddles, but there the re-
spondent must work not only with basically uncoded data but also with data
that are kinesic and therefore highly ephemeral.
Joking riddles are the second category of forms that are extremely
difficult of solution. To be sure, joking routines can differ in the quality of
their humor, but, generally speaking, the propositions serve primarily as a
setup for the punch line of the answer:
36. What is the worst weather for rats and mice? --when it's rain-
ing cats and dogs. (Leventhal and Cray 1963, 254)
37. What's tall and says eef eif [eof] muf? -a backward giant.
(McCosh 1976, 176)
THE PARODIC IMPULSE
Parodic forms are not so much a distinct category of riddle as they are forms
that extend the humor of joking riddles into absurdity and nonsense. Pa-
rodic riddles adopt the organizational strategies of more conservative forms.
i8z RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? What marks them as extensions is the degree of violence they do to assump-
tions of everyday life.
Parodic riddles are very popular among urban children, and various
researchers have commented on this fascination. Alto Jablow and Carl With-
ers (1965) view the riddles as safety-valve mechanisms that enable young-
sters to express their amused, angry, or frightened awareness of the fast-
paced, violent, and often irrational world in which they live. If the routines
are absurd and meaningless, Jablow and Withers argue, it is because urban
society itself reflects those characteristics (1965, 252-55). For their part,
Mary and Herbert Knapp (1976) agree that children may become connois-
seurs of chaos in buffering themselves against modern life. The Knapps,
however, also advance an alternate explanation. Urban children, they sug-
gest, have developed sophisticated tastes in their folk humor. Because pa-
rodic forms deal in ambiguity and nonsense, they speak to children's almost
insatiable fascination with complex and startling relationships (1976, 111).
Some parodic riddles can be used to challenge assumptions about
what is possible in the everyday world. We can understand how an answer
fits its question, yet what the riddle as a whole proposes is nonsensical given
a conventional understanding of the "real" world. If what these riddles sug-
gest were valid, we would need to reorganize our conception of the world
around us. Examples of parodic forms include the following:12
Description:
38. What's big and grey with a purple spot in the middle? -an
elephant hit by a Comanche grape. (Weiner 1970, 32)
39. What's green and flies from planet to planet? -an interplan-
etary cucumber. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 110)
40. (Snap your fingers in the air several times, jerking your hand
up and down. ) "What's this? "-a butterfly with hiccups (or, a
drunk butterfly). (Leventhal and Cray 1963, 249)
41. FIG. A clam with buck teeth (Roemer, unpublished data).
183
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? Contrast:
42. What's the difference between unloading dead babies and un-
loading bowling balls? -you can't use a pitchfork to unload bowl-
ing balls. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 111)
Classification/Definition:
43. What do you call a werewolf in a Dacron suit? --a wash and
werewolf. (Knapp and Knapp 1976, 110)
Effect/Cause:
44. Why do elephants have flat feet? -from jumping out of palm
trees. (McCosh 1976, 224)
Catch Riddles
Using metacommunicative techniques, other riddles violate the conventions
of riddling as a particular type of social exchange. No. 45 below challenges
the expectation that a riddler's question will provide information relevant
to the answer. Here, the respondents receive only part of the formulaic in-
formation characteristic of contrast riddles. Beyond that, the respondents are
on their own:
45. A: What's the difference?
B: Between what?
A: On this one I'm giving you no clues. (Jablow and Withers
1965, 249)
Other riddles are iconoclastic in other ways. No. 46 begins with a descrip-
tive question, the conventional way of initiating some of the most conser-
vative of English-language riddles. It concludes, however, by challenging the
assumption that riddlers know the answer to the questions they pose:
46. What's red, purple, green, yellow, gray, purple, sky-blue, and
green?
-I don't know, that's why I'm asking you. (McCosh 1976, 178)
A related surprise awaits respondents with no. 47. In addition to not sup-
184 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? plying a satisfactory resolution, the riddle threatens the respondent's assump-
tion of physical security:
47. A: What has six legs, fuzzy ears, and a long tail?
B: What?
A: I don't know, but there's one on your back. (McCosh 1976,
178)
In this and other catch riddles (Roemer 1977), the respondents are set up
for victimization.
By cooperating with what they think is the first speaker's
intent, the respondents place themselves unknowingly in a vulnerable posi-
tion. The trickster capitalizes on this vulnerability at the riddle's conclusion:
Description:
48. What word starts with F and ends with CK? -firetruck.
(Winslow 1966c, 172)
Contrast:
49. A: What's the difference between an egg and an elephant?
B: I don't know, what?
A: If you don't know, I'll never send you to get eggs from the
shop. (McCosh 1976, 178)
Cause-Effect
50. If you threw a white ball into a black sea, what would it be-
come? -wet. (McCosh 1976, 181)
THE ACQUISITION OF RIDDLE COMPETENCE
Though research is still in its early stages, we have found that youngsters
do not learn to riddle simply by memorizing and repeating set pieces. In-
stead, their competence proceeds through several stages and includes knowl-
edge of riddle production as well as of riddle interaction. Below, I review
this acquisition process and, reflecting the bias in the literature, focus on
urban American children.
MADE-UP ROUTINES
Youngsters acquire much of their knowledge of riddling in actual riddle in-
185
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? teraction. Some exchanges may be conducted with adults; most often, they
occur with peers. In American society, the peer group represents the single
most important laboratory for youngsters' acquisition and exercise of rid-
dling competence. 14
When exposed to older children's riddling, young children (of around
five years) notice question-answer sequences that call special attention to the
asker. Although they find this sort of performance appealing, young children
usually possess no ready stock of riddles to draw on. Wanting to participate,
they make up their own, trying to imitate those they have heard. As I've
shown above, riddles are based in common solicitational strategies. Young
children depend on these strategies in making up their routines. Among
McDowell's informants (1979, 59-68), the most frequently adopted strate-
gies were those of cause and effect and description:
Causality:
51. How come the pig likes to get in the mud ? -'cause he likes to
take a bath in the mud. (McDowell 1979, 245)
Description:
52. What's red and white, and doesn't do nothing, and has a stick
down its side, and the red and white thing is against the stick? -a
flag. (McDowell 1979, 245)
By far, descriptive routines (like that in no. 52) appear to outnumber those
based in other strategies (Weiner 1970; McDowell 1979).
Made-up descriptive routines enumerate the features or actions of
some object. " Often, the referenced object or some reproduction of it is
within the riddler's immediate environment. Like made-up routines based
on other strategies, those of description do not contain a block element. In-
stead, the ideal routine-according to peer group standards (McDowell
1979)-is accurate and transparent, its answer easy to grasp. The
respondent's correct answer signals that a "good" question has been asked:
53. What's big and has black stripes and white stripes? -a zebra.
(U. T. Children's Folklore Project, unpublished data)16
54. What's real big and it grows in the ground and it's got leaves
on it? -a tree. (U. T. Children's Folklore Project, unpublished data)
186 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? There is some evidence of cultural and regional differences in children's
made-up descriptive routines. With respect to cultural differences, we can
compare examples 53 and 54 above with those below. The former were
obtained from Austin Anglo youngsters; the following, from Austin Mexi-
can American children:
55. It's a little circle in your stomach. -belly button. (McDowell
1979, 244)
56. It's in a hole, what do you call it, in the zoo? -a guinea pig.
(McDowell 1979, 244)
Though children of both groups used solicitations phrased as interrogatives,
only the Mexican American youngsters in addition employed declarative
solicitations (nos. 55, 56 above). This suggests that children from different
cultural backgrounds may exploit linguistic resources differently in their
riddle-making. These differences are suggested when we contrast material
from the Austin youngsters (both Anglo and Mexican American) with ma-
terial obtained in Massachusetts by Meryl Weiner (1970). Like the Austin
children, the Massachusetts youngsters most often relied on the interroga-
tive format. The youngest children in Weiner's sample, however, occasion-
ally framed their descriptions as first-person statements. The riddler pretends
to adopt the identity of the person, place, or thing being described:'7
57. I have a tail. I have a body. I have a face. I am white and
black. What am I?
-a skunk. (Weiner 1970, 9)
This technique does not appear in the Austin material.
THE ACQUISITION OF RIDDLING COMPETENCE
The acquisition of riddling skills is essentially the acquisition of an artistic
competence. Children just entering elementary school have mastered the
basic linguistic resources of their native tongue. They have a rudimentary
grasp of the cognitive systems in their culture. And, through their sponta-
neous play, they have gained practice with the "performance persona"
(McDowell 1979, 187). In learning how to riddle, children learn to apply
these acquired and developing competencies to the specific purposes of riddle
interaction.
Although a number of developmental schemes are available (for ex-
187
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? ample, Weiner 1970, Sanches and Kirshenblatt-Gimblett 1976, Sutton-
Smith 1976b; Wolfenstein 1954), that proposed by McDowell (1979) and
elaborated by Bauman (1977b) best suits our purposes here. In contrast
to the others, this perspective treats riddle acquisition as a social interac-
tional process.
During the initial stage in the acquisition process (around five years),
youngsters learn the basics of riddle interaction. Through observing and
participating, they learn that the poser of a riddle gains the floor, that the
riddle contains both a question and an answer, that the respondent should
be given a chance to guess, and that the poser should know the answer to
his own question (Bauman 1977b, 26). Guided by these observations and
stimulated by the riddles they hear from others, these youngsters make up
their own routines (as discussed below); they also learn that the respondent's
correct answer reflects positively on the riddler. By learning to present such
"successful" routines, young children increase their understanding of the
relationship between riddling and ego-enhancement.
At the second stage (around six years), children tend to modify their
views as to what constitutes a "good" riddle. Now they come to believe that
a riddle is a puzzling question with an arbitrary answer (Bauman 1977b,
27; Sutton-Smith 1976b, 115). Noticing that riddlers always seem to be right,
regardless of the esoteric sequences they come up with, children at this stage
are apt to initiate sequences like the following:
58. riddler: What color is blood?
respondent: Red
riddler: Nope, it's blue and black. (Bauman 1977b, 27)
"Potent elicitations" (Bauman 1977b, 27) such as this don't receive much
peer group reinforcement. Since the relationship between the question and
its answer is grasped only by the riddler (if indeed that), coparticipants aren't
motivated to encourage similar routines. Faced with losing respondents,
young riddlers once again modify their understanding of riddles. Though they
still regard riddles as confusing, children come to assume that riddles must
nevertheless be entertaining. As a result, they turn to the subjects of sexual-
ity and scatology. If they weren't aware of it before, they soon realize that
"dirt" sells:
59. riddler: What do you call people?
respondent: I don't know, what?
riddler: Doo-doo people. (Bauman 1977b, 27)
188 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? Toward the end of the second stage, children's interest in traditional riddles
begins to surface. This burgeoning recognition, however, outstrips the young-
sters' understanding of riddles' speech play. The children can't yet grasp all
of the linguistic and sociolinguistic complexities in the riddles they try to
repeat. As a result, some of their attempts are flawed:
60. What did the mean frog say to the nice frog? -"I hope you
crick" (instead of "croak"). (Bauman 1977b, 28)
As children enter and proceed through the third stage (of around seven
years), their production errors decrease markedly. Youngsters come not only
to recognize the riddle as a traditional form, they are also beginning to ap-
preciate it as such. As this shift is realized, the children increase their efforts
to learn and to produce riddles as preset pieces, with a stable question and
a fixed answer. The display of competence becomes a matter of demonstrat-
ing one's knowledge of riddles within the peer group. '8
In the final stage (at around eight years), children routinely succeed in
the use of traditional riddles. In effect, they have gained mastery of peer-group
riddling. The literature (Wolfenstein 1954; McDowell 1979) suggests that
children maintain an interest in riddling until about the age of ten or twelve.
After that time, they put their performance energies into other genres, allow-
ing their interest in riddling to decline. Since most mainstream American so-
ciety tends to regard riddling as a children's activity and thus as one inappro-
priate for adults except under special circumstances (Roberts and Forman
1972, 182), we might say that youngsters between eight and twelve have prob-
ably acquired as much competence in riddling as they will ever have.
Functions of Children's Riddling
Outside of the fact that it allows its participants to engage in social interac-
tion, there is perhaps no single, universal function of riddling. At the least,
riddle functions result from an interplay among the participants' perception
of situational circumstances, their combined and individual goals, and their
relative commitment to group and community standards. Documenting
riddle functions depends on in-depth fieldwork and the analysis of riddles
in individual communities, groups, and situations.
Below, I review some of the possible riddle functions based on the
factor of social interaction.
Several researchers (for example, Roberts and Forman 1971; Sutton-
Smith 1976b; Bauman 1982) have observed that in many urban groups rid-
dling is a way of engaging in, representing, and commenting on the processes
189
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? of questioning and answering. '9 Children have an interest in playing out and
playing with interrogative formats. For example, riddling models the kind
of interrogation that is ubiquitous in educational settings (Bauman 1982,
184). As a result, children's riddling may in part be an adaptive mechanism,
allowing youngsters to come to terms with the participant structures of
schooling. This perspective has several points that support it. Adult-child
interrogation is characteristic not only of classroom interaction but also of
interaction in the home environment. Parents routinely query children about
the children's activities. It is not surprising, then, that children are attracted
to an activity that allows them to serve in the role of interrogator. That role
allows children some access to power. The children also become the focus
of attention. And, in supplying the riddle answer, they enjoy "being right. "
As an expressive model of interrogation, riddling also permits young-
sters to manipulate the resources of communication. As I've shown, riddles
are based on common solicitational strategies. As framing devices, these strat-
egies serve as tools by which people orient and indicate their perceptions of
the world. In riddling, such strategies facilitate the exchange of information,
for example, by advising respondents of some of the cognitive and commu-
nicative work they are to do. Descriptive questions require that respondents
notice details and generalize from them. Respondents to classification ques-
tions must indicate the categorization of experience. Comparison-contrast
questions require information about similarities or differences in domains
of experience. To be sure, riddles counterpoint these familiar strategies with
a variety of disruptive techniques. True riddles have their block elements;
joking riddles surprise with humor, and parodic forms startle with nonsense
or victimization. These techniques complicate communication, whether the
respondent's task is to solve the riddler's question or to relate its answer to
assumptions of everyday life . The tension, however, is not irresoluble. The
enigma of a true riddle can be solved, the humor of a joking riddle can be
appreciated, and (except for the most drastic of the victimization forms) the
absurdity of a parodic riddle can be dismissed as inconsequential to every-
day concerns.
In concluding, I should point out that children are not likely to be
self-consciously aware of the functions summarized above. For them, rid-
dling is primarily a form of folk entertainment, one that also attests to the
participants' competence in peer group and community traditions.
NOTES TO CHAPTER EIGHT
1. Catch routines, soliciting either a verbal or a physical response, are treated
190 RIDDLES
? ? Generated for (University of Chicago) on 2014-12-24 15:03 GMT / http://hdl. handle. net/2027/usu. 39060010034923 Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives / http://www. hathitrust. org/access_use#cc-by-nc-nd-3. 0
? ? in Roemer 1977. The interactional organization and developmental acquisition of
knock-knock routines are discussed in Bauman 1977b. For an extensive collection of
knock-knock routines, see McCosh 1976, 204-12.
2. The riddle proposition is the "question" unit of a riddle.
