A
magnificent
and
timely work.
timely work.
Steven-Pinker-The-Blank-Slate 1
?
{i} >> PENGUIN BOOKS
THE BLANK SLATE
Steven Pinker is Johnstone Professor of Psychology at Harvard University. His research on visual cognition and the psychology of language has earned prizes from the National Academy of Sciences and the American Psychological Association. Pinker has also received many awards for his teaching at MIT and for his books How the Mind Works (which was also a finalist for the Pulitzer Prize) and The Language Instinct. He is an elected fellow of several scientific societies, associate editor of Cognition, and a member of the usage panel of the American Heritage Dictionary. He has written for The New York Times, Time, The New Yorker, The New Republic, Slate, and Technology Review.
~
Praise for The Blank Slate
"A brilliant and forceful summary. . . A well-informed and well-written account of [human] limitations, [written with] a graceful interleaving of scientific and literary sources. . . . [This] fine book helps with a task that we all must begin to
? ? ? take seriously. . . Can it be that we have finally grown up? "
-- Melvin Konner, The American Prospect
"This is a brilliant book. It is beautifully written, and addresses profound issues with courage and clarity. There is nothing else like it, and it is going to have an impact that extends well beyond the scientific academy. "
-- Paul Bloom, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
"Steven Pinker has written an extremely good book -- clear, well argued, fair, learned, tough, witty, humane, stimulating. I only hope that people study it carefully before rising up ideologically against him. If they do, they will see that the idea of an innately flawed but wonderfully rich human nature is a force for good, not evil.
-- Colin McGinn, The Washington Post
"Steven Pinker is a man of encyclopedic knowledge and an incisive style of argument. His argument in The Blank Slate is that intellectual life in the West, and much of our social and political policy, was increasingly dominated through the twentieth century by a view of human nature that is fundamentally flawed; that this domination has been backed by something that amounts to academic terrorism (he does not put it quite so strongly): and that we would benefit {ii} substantially from a more realistic view. Pinker's exposition is thoroughly readable and of enviable clarity. His explanation of such a difficult technical matter as the analysis of variance and regression in twin studies, for example, would be very hard to better. He is not afraid of using strong language. . . in addition, parts of the book are delightfully funny. "
-- John R. G. Turner, The Times Literary Supplement
"Anyone who has read Pinker's earlier books -- including How the Mind Works and The Language Instinct -- will rightly guess that his latest effort is similarly sweeping, erudite, sharply argued, richly footnoted and fun to read. It's
? also highly persuasive. "
-- Michael Lemonick, Time
"[Pinker] makes his main argument persuasively and with great verve. . .
The Blank Slate ought to be read by anybody who feels they have had enough of nature-nurture rows or who thinks they already know where they stand on the science wars. It could change their minds. . . If nothing else, Mr. Pinker's book is a wonderfully readable taster of new research, much of it ingenious, designed to show that many more of our emotional biases and mental aptitudes than previously thought are hard-wired or, to use the old word, innate. . . This is a breath of air for a topic that has been politicized for too long. "
-- The Economist
"[Pinker] wades resolutely into the comforting gloom surrounding these not quite forbidden topics and calmly, lucidly marshals the facts to ground his strikingly subversive Darwinian claims -- subversive not of any of the things we properly hold dear but subversive of the phony protective layers of misinformation surrounding them. . . My reservations with Pinker's view [will be resolved] in the bright light of rational inquiry that he brings to these
important topics. "
-- Dan Dennett, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate brilliantly delineates the current state of play in the nature-nurture debate. Read it to understand not just the moral and aesthetic blindness of your friends, but the misguided idealism of nations. A magnificent and
timely work. "
-- Fay Weldon, The Daily Telegraph
"[Pinker] points us in the direction of a more productive debate, a debate in which the implications of science are confronted forthrightly and not simply wished away by politicized scientists. "
-- Francis Fukuyama, The Wall Street Journal {iii}
"The Blank Slate is. . . a stylish piece of work. I won't say it is better than The Language Instinct or How the Mind Works, but it is as good -- which is very high praise indeed. What a superb thinker and writer he is: what a role model to young scientists. And how courageous to buck the liberal trend in science, while remaining in person the best sort of liberal. Pinker is a star, and the world of science is lucky to have him. "
-- Richard Dawkins, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate is not dismal at all, but unexpectedly bracing. It feels a bit like being burgled. You're shocked, your things are gone, but you can't help thinking about how you're going to replace them. What Steven Pinker has done is
? break into our common human home and steal our illusions. "
-- John Morrish, The Independent
"As a brightly lighted path between what we would like to believe and what we need to know, [The Blank Slate] is required reading. Pinker presents an unanswerable case for accepting that man can be, as he is, both wired and free. "
-- Frederic Raphael, Los Angeles Times
"Pinker's thinking and writing are first-rate; maybe even better than that. The Blank Slate is much-needed, long overdue and -- if you are interested in what might be called the 'human nature wars' -- somewhere between that old standby, 'required reading,' and downright indispensable. It is unlikely to change the minds of those who are rigidly committed to the blank slate perspective, but for anyone whose 'nature' includes even a modicum of open- mindedness, it should prove a revelation. "
-- David Barash, Human Nature Review "Pinker is one of those rare writers who is at once persuasive and comprehensive, informative and entertaining. "
-- Kevin Shapiro, Commentary
"The fight for a separation of politics from science is an eminently sensible, logical, and ultimately humanistic task, and it took someone as brave as Pinker to dedicate himself to it. . . . [This is a] necessary book, a book that in a more truthful intellectual climate -- one open to the idea that any knowledge about ourselves can only enhance our ability to act well and compassionately -- would not have had to be written. In this climate, however, we should be grateful that it was. "
-- Daniel Smith, The Boston Globe {iv}
"The Blank Slate deserves to be read carefully and with an open mind. . . This landmark book makes an important contribution to the argument about nature vs. nurture in humans. Whether or not most readers end up on Pinker's side of the fence, one can hope that his thoroughness and reasoning will shed light into the darker corners where research has been suppressed by taboos, and where freedom of thought and speech have been inhibited by fear of consequences for asking forbidden questions. "
-- Nancy Jeannette Friedlander, The San Diego Union-Tribune
"This book is a modern magnum opus. The scholarship alone is mind-boggling, a monument of careful research, meticulous citation, breadth of input from diverse fields, great writing and humor. "
-- Tom Paskal, The Montreal Gazette "A delightfully provocative read. . . A constantly dynamic, if tacit, exchange between the author and his readers. "
-- Patrick Watson, The Globe and Mail
"A feast of a book. Pinker's analytical and impish mind ranges from Charles Darwin to Abigail Van Buren, from scientific studies to Annie Hall. . . . It will be a rare reader who agrees with everything in this book. But it is an
? intelligent book that says what it means and thinks about what it is saying. . . . Though much of the book is about human differences, the bigger idea is inherited similarity -- the 'psychological unity of our species. ' It is not a blank slate but a slate with a face -- a face that might be called human nature. When Pinker starts describing it, the reader will surely recognize it. "
{v}
THE BLANK SLATE
The Modern Denial of Human Nature
Steven Pinker
PENGUIN BOOKS
-- Bruce Ramsey, The Seattle Times
? ? ? ? ? ? <<
{vi} >> To Don, Judy, Leda, and John
PENGUIN BOOKS
Published by the Penguin Group
Penguin Group (USA) Inc. , 375 Hudson Street, New York, New York 10014, U. S. A.
Penguin Books Ltd, 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
Penguin Books Australia Ltd, 250 Camberwell Road, Camberwell, Victoria 3124, Australia Penguin Books Canada Ltd, 10 Alcorn Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M4V 3B2
Penguin Books India (P) Ltd, 11 Community Centre, Panchsheel Park, New Delhi - 110 017, India Penguin Books (N. Z. ) Ltd, Cnr Rosedale and Airborne Roads, Albany, Auckland, New Zealand Penguin Books (South Africa) (Pty) Ltd, 24 Sturdee Avenue, Rosebank, Johannesburg 2196, South Africa
Penguin Books Ltd, Registered Offices: 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
First published in the United States of America by Viking Penguin, a member of Penguin Putnam Inc. 2002
Published in Penguin Books 2003
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Copyright (C) Steven Pinker, 2002 All rights reserved
? ? ? ? Grateful acknowledgment is made for permission to reprint the following copyrighted material. Page 22: Lyrics from "A Simple Desultory Philippic (or How I Was Robert McNamara'd into Submission)"; copyright (C) 1965, Paul Simon; used by permission of the publisher: Paul Simon Music. Page 57: Chart, "Percentage of Male Deaths Caused by Warfare," from War Before Civilization by Lawrence H. Keeley, copyright (C) 1996 by Oxford University Press, Inc. ; used by permission of Oxford University Press, Inc. Page 88: Diagram of the wiring of the primate visual system from Michael Gazzaniga, The Cognitive Neurosciences, The MIT Press (1996). Page 179: Lyrics from "Gee, Officer Krupke" by Leonard Bernstein & Stephen Sondheim; (C) 1956, Amberson Holdings LLC and Stephen Sondheim; copyright renewed; Leonard Bernstein Music
Publishing Company LLC, publisher; used by permission. Page 199: Diagram, "Turning the Tables," from Mind Sights by Roger N. Shepard, (C) 1990 by Roger N. Shepard; reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and Company, LLC. "Checker Shadow Illusion" (C) Edward Adelson, 2002; reprinted with permission. Page 326: Lyrics from "You Don't Mess Around with Jim," written by Jim Croce; (C) 1972 (renewed), Time in a Bottle/Croce Publishing (ASCAP); all rights reserved; used by
permission.
THE LIBRARY OF CONGRESS HAS CATALOGUED THE HARDCOVER EDITION AS FOLLOWS: Pinker, Steven, 1954-
The blank slate : the modern denial of human nature / Steven Pinker.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and index.
ISBN 0-670-03151-8 (hc. )
ISBN 0 14 20. 0334 4 (pbk. )
1. Nature and nurture. I. Title.
BF341. P47 2002 155. 2'34 -- dc21 2002022719
Printed in the United States of America Set in Minion
Except in the United States of America, this book is sold subject to the condition that it shall not, by way of trade or otherwise, be lent, re-sold, hired out, or otherwise circulated without the publisher's prior consent in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition including this condition being imposed on the subsequent
purchaser.
The scanning, uploading and distribution of this book via the Internet or via any other means without the permission of the publisher is illegal and punishable by law. Please purchase only authorized electronic editions, and do not participate in or encourage electronic piracy of copyrighted materials. Your support of the author's rights is appreciated.
<< {vii} >> PREFACE
"Not another book on nature and nurture! Are there really people out there who still believe that the mind is a blank slate? Isn't it obvious to anyone with more than one child, to anyone who has been in a heterosexual relationship, or to anyone who has noticed that children learn language but house pets don't, that people are born with certain talents and temperaments? Haven't we all moved beyond the simplistic dichotomy between heredity and environment and realized that all behavior comes out of an interaction between the two? "
This is the kind of reaction I got from colleagues when I explained my plans for this book. At first glance the reaction is not unreasonable. Maybe nature versus nurture is a dead issue. Anyone familiar with current writings on mind and behavior has seen claims to the middle ground like these:
If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with this issue. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.
This is not going to be one of those books that says everything is genetic: it isn't. The environment is just as important as the genes. The things children experience while they are growing up are just as important as the things they are born with.
Even when a behavior is heritable, an individual's behavior is still a product of development, and thus it has a causal environmental component. . . . The modern understanding of how phenotypes are inherited through the replication of both genetic and environmental {viii} conditions suggests that. . . cultural traditions -- behaviors copied by children from their parents -- are likely to be crucial.
If you think these are innocuous compromises that show that everyone has outgrown the nature-nurture debate, think again. The quotations come, in fact, from three of the most incendiary books of the last decade. The first is from The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, who argue that the difference in average IQ scores between American blacks and American whites has both genetic and environmental causes. 1 The second is from The Nurture Assumption by Judith Rich Harris, who argues that children's personalities are shaped by their genes as well as by
? ? ? ? ? ? ? their environments, so similarities between children and their parents may come from their shared genes and not just from the effects of parenting. 2 The third is from A Natural History of Rape by Randy Thornhill and Craig Palmer,
who argue that rape is not simply a product of culture but also has roots in the nature of men's sexuality. 3 For invoking nurture and nature, not nurture alone, these authors have been picketed, shouted down, subjected to searing invective in the press, even denounced in Congress. Others expressing such opinions have been censored, assaulted, or threatened with criminal prosecution. 4
The idea that nature and nurture interact to shape some part of the mind might turn out to be wrong, but it is not wishy-washy or unexceptionable, even in the twenty-first century, thousands of years after the issue was framed. When it comes to explaining human thought and behavior, the possibility that heredity plays any role at all still has the power to shock. To acknowledge human nature, many think, is to endorse racism, sexism, war, greed, genocide, nihilism, reactionary politics, and neglect of children and the disadvantaged. Any claim that the mind has an innate organization strikes people not as a hypothesis that might be incorrect but as a thought it is immoral to think.
This book is about the moral, emotional, and political colorings of the concept of human nature in modern life. I will retrace the history that led people to see human nature as a dangerous idea, and I will try to unsnarl the moral and political rat's nests that have entangled the idea along the way. Though no book on human nature can hope to be uncontroversial, I did not write it to be yet another "explosive" book, as dust jackets tend to say.
A magnificent and
timely work. "
-- Fay Weldon, The Daily Telegraph
"[Pinker] points us in the direction of a more productive debate, a debate in which the implications of science are confronted forthrightly and not simply wished away by politicized scientists. "
-- Francis Fukuyama, The Wall Street Journal {iii}
"The Blank Slate is. . . a stylish piece of work. I won't say it is better than The Language Instinct or How the Mind Works, but it is as good -- which is very high praise indeed. What a superb thinker and writer he is: what a role model to young scientists. And how courageous to buck the liberal trend in science, while remaining in person the best sort of liberal. Pinker is a star, and the world of science is lucky to have him. "
-- Richard Dawkins, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate is not dismal at all, but unexpectedly bracing. It feels a bit like being burgled. You're shocked, your things are gone, but you can't help thinking about how you're going to replace them. What Steven Pinker has done is
? break into our common human home and steal our illusions. "
-- John Morrish, The Independent
"As a brightly lighted path between what we would like to believe and what we need to know, [The Blank Slate] is required reading. Pinker presents an unanswerable case for accepting that man can be, as he is, both wired and free. "
-- Frederic Raphael, Los Angeles Times
"Pinker's thinking and writing are first-rate; maybe even better than that. The Blank Slate is much-needed, long overdue and -- if you are interested in what might be called the 'human nature wars' -- somewhere between that old standby, 'required reading,' and downright indispensable. It is unlikely to change the minds of those who are rigidly committed to the blank slate perspective, but for anyone whose 'nature' includes even a modicum of open- mindedness, it should prove a revelation. "
-- David Barash, Human Nature Review "Pinker is one of those rare writers who is at once persuasive and comprehensive, informative and entertaining. "
-- Kevin Shapiro, Commentary
"The fight for a separation of politics from science is an eminently sensible, logical, and ultimately humanistic task, and it took someone as brave as Pinker to dedicate himself to it. . . . [This is a] necessary book, a book that in a more truthful intellectual climate -- one open to the idea that any knowledge about ourselves can only enhance our ability to act well and compassionately -- would not have had to be written. In this climate, however, we should be grateful that it was. "
-- Daniel Smith, The Boston Globe {iv}
"The Blank Slate deserves to be read carefully and with an open mind. . . This landmark book makes an important contribution to the argument about nature vs. nurture in humans. Whether or not most readers end up on Pinker's side of the fence, one can hope that his thoroughness and reasoning will shed light into the darker corners where research has been suppressed by taboos, and where freedom of thought and speech have been inhibited by fear of consequences for asking forbidden questions. "
-- Nancy Jeannette Friedlander, The San Diego Union-Tribune
"This book is a modern magnum opus. The scholarship alone is mind-boggling, a monument of careful research, meticulous citation, breadth of input from diverse fields, great writing and humor. "
-- Tom Paskal, The Montreal Gazette "A delightfully provocative read. . . A constantly dynamic, if tacit, exchange between the author and his readers. "
-- Patrick Watson, The Globe and Mail
"A feast of a book. Pinker's analytical and impish mind ranges from Charles Darwin to Abigail Van Buren, from scientific studies to Annie Hall. . . . It will be a rare reader who agrees with everything in this book. But it is an
? intelligent book that says what it means and thinks about what it is saying. . . . Though much of the book is about human differences, the bigger idea is inherited similarity -- the 'psychological unity of our species. ' It is not a blank slate but a slate with a face -- a face that might be called human nature. When Pinker starts describing it, the reader will surely recognize it. "
{v}
THE BLANK SLATE
The Modern Denial of Human Nature
Steven Pinker
PENGUIN BOOKS
-- Bruce Ramsey, The Seattle Times
? ? ? ? ? ? <<
{vi} >> To Don, Judy, Leda, and John
PENGUIN BOOKS
Published by the Penguin Group
Penguin Group (USA) Inc. , 375 Hudson Street, New York, New York 10014, U. S. A.
Penguin Books Ltd, 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
Penguin Books Australia Ltd, 250 Camberwell Road, Camberwell, Victoria 3124, Australia Penguin Books Canada Ltd, 10 Alcorn Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M4V 3B2
Penguin Books India (P) Ltd, 11 Community Centre, Panchsheel Park, New Delhi - 110 017, India Penguin Books (N. Z. ) Ltd, Cnr Rosedale and Airborne Roads, Albany, Auckland, New Zealand Penguin Books (South Africa) (Pty) Ltd, 24 Sturdee Avenue, Rosebank, Johannesburg 2196, South Africa
Penguin Books Ltd, Registered Offices: 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
First published in the United States of America by Viking Penguin, a member of Penguin Putnam Inc. 2002
Published in Penguin Books 2003
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Copyright (C) Steven Pinker, 2002 All rights reserved
? ? ? ? Grateful acknowledgment is made for permission to reprint the following copyrighted material. Page 22: Lyrics from "A Simple Desultory Philippic (or How I Was Robert McNamara'd into Submission)"; copyright (C) 1965, Paul Simon; used by permission of the publisher: Paul Simon Music. Page 57: Chart, "Percentage of Male Deaths Caused by Warfare," from War Before Civilization by Lawrence H. Keeley, copyright (C) 1996 by Oxford University Press, Inc. ; used by permission of Oxford University Press, Inc. Page 88: Diagram of the wiring of the primate visual system from Michael Gazzaniga, The Cognitive Neurosciences, The MIT Press (1996). Page 179: Lyrics from "Gee, Officer Krupke" by Leonard Bernstein & Stephen Sondheim; (C) 1956, Amberson Holdings LLC and Stephen Sondheim; copyright renewed; Leonard Bernstein Music
Publishing Company LLC, publisher; used by permission. Page 199: Diagram, "Turning the Tables," from Mind Sights by Roger N. Shepard, (C) 1990 by Roger N. Shepard; reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and Company, LLC. "Checker Shadow Illusion" (C) Edward Adelson, 2002; reprinted with permission. Page 326: Lyrics from "You Don't Mess Around with Jim," written by Jim Croce; (C) 1972 (renewed), Time in a Bottle/Croce Publishing (ASCAP); all rights reserved; used by
permission.
THE LIBRARY OF CONGRESS HAS CATALOGUED THE HARDCOVER EDITION AS FOLLOWS: Pinker, Steven, 1954-
The blank slate : the modern denial of human nature / Steven Pinker.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and index.
ISBN 0-670-03151-8 (hc. )
ISBN 0 14 20. 0334 4 (pbk. )
1. Nature and nurture. I. Title.
BF341. P47 2002 155. 2'34 -- dc21 2002022719
Printed in the United States of America Set in Minion
Except in the United States of America, this book is sold subject to the condition that it shall not, by way of trade or otherwise, be lent, re-sold, hired out, or otherwise circulated without the publisher's prior consent in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition including this condition being imposed on the subsequent
purchaser.
The scanning, uploading and distribution of this book via the Internet or via any other means without the permission of the publisher is illegal and punishable by law. Please purchase only authorized electronic editions, and do not participate in or encourage electronic piracy of copyrighted materials. Your support of the author's rights is appreciated.
<< {vii} >> PREFACE
"Not another book on nature and nurture! Are there really people out there who still believe that the mind is a blank slate? Isn't it obvious to anyone with more than one child, to anyone who has been in a heterosexual relationship, or to anyone who has noticed that children learn language but house pets don't, that people are born with certain talents and temperaments? Haven't we all moved beyond the simplistic dichotomy between heredity and environment and realized that all behavior comes out of an interaction between the two? "
This is the kind of reaction I got from colleagues when I explained my plans for this book. At first glance the reaction is not unreasonable. Maybe nature versus nurture is a dead issue. Anyone familiar with current writings on mind and behavior has seen claims to the middle ground like these:
If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with this issue. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.
This is not going to be one of those books that says everything is genetic: it isn't. The environment is just as important as the genes. The things children experience while they are growing up are just as important as the things they are born with.
Even when a behavior is heritable, an individual's behavior is still a product of development, and thus it has a causal environmental component. . . . The modern understanding of how phenotypes are inherited through the replication of both genetic and environmental {viii} conditions suggests that. . . cultural traditions -- behaviors copied by children from their parents -- are likely to be crucial.
If you think these are innocuous compromises that show that everyone has outgrown the nature-nurture debate, think again. The quotations come, in fact, from three of the most incendiary books of the last decade. The first is from The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, who argue that the difference in average IQ scores between American blacks and American whites has both genetic and environmental causes. 1 The second is from The Nurture Assumption by Judith Rich Harris, who argues that children's personalities are shaped by their genes as well as by
? ? ? ? ? ? ? their environments, so similarities between children and their parents may come from their shared genes and not just from the effects of parenting. 2 The third is from A Natural History of Rape by Randy Thornhill and Craig Palmer,
who argue that rape is not simply a product of culture but also has roots in the nature of men's sexuality. 3 For invoking nurture and nature, not nurture alone, these authors have been picketed, shouted down, subjected to searing invective in the press, even denounced in Congress. Others expressing such opinions have been censored, assaulted, or threatened with criminal prosecution. 4
The idea that nature and nurture interact to shape some part of the mind might turn out to be wrong, but it is not wishy-washy or unexceptionable, even in the twenty-first century, thousands of years after the issue was framed. When it comes to explaining human thought and behavior, the possibility that heredity plays any role at all still has the power to shock. To acknowledge human nature, many think, is to endorse racism, sexism, war, greed, genocide, nihilism, reactionary politics, and neglect of children and the disadvantaged. Any claim that the mind has an innate organization strikes people not as a hypothesis that might be incorrect but as a thought it is immoral to think.
This book is about the moral, emotional, and political colorings of the concept of human nature in modern life. I will retrace the history that led people to see human nature as a dangerous idea, and I will try to unsnarl the moral and political rat's nests that have entangled the idea along the way. Though no book on human nature can hope to be uncontroversial, I did not write it to be yet another "explosive" book, as dust jackets tend to say. I am not, as many people assume, countering an extreme "nurture" position with an extreme "nature" position, with the truth lying somewhere in between. In some cases, an extreme environmentalist explanation is correct: which language you speak is an obvious example, and differences among races and ethnic groups in test scores may be another. In other cases, such as certain inherited neurological disorders, an extreme hereditarian explanation is correct. In most cases the correct explanation will invoke a complex interaction between heredity and environment: culture is crucial, but culture could not exist without mental {ix} faculties that allow humans to create and learn culture to begin with. My goal in this book is not to argue that genes are everything and culture is nothing -- no one believes that -- but to explore why the extreme position (that culture is everything) is so often seen as moderate, and the moderate position is seen as extreme.
Nor does acknowledging human nature have the political implications so many fear. It does not, for example, require one to abandon feminism, or to accept current levels of inequality or violence, or to treat morality as a fiction. For the most part I will try not to advocate particular policies or to advance the agenda of the political left or right. I believe that controversies about policy almost always involve tradeoffs between competing values, and that science is equipped to identify the tradeoffs but not to resolve them. Many of these tradeoffs, I will show, arise from features of human nature, and by clarifying them I hope to make our collective choices, whatever they are, better informed. If I am an advocate, it is for discoveries about human nature that have been ignored or suppressed in modern discussions of human affairs.
Why is it important to sort this all out? The refusal to acknowledge human nature is like the Victorians' embarrassment about sex, only worse: it distorts our science and scholarship, our public discourse, and our day-to- day lives. Logicians tell us that a single contradiction can corrupt a set of statements and allow falsehoods to proliferate through it. The dogma that human nature does not exist, in the face of evidence from science and common sense that it does, is just such a corrupting influence.
First, the doctrine that the mind is a blank slate has distorted the study of human beings, and thus the public and private decisions that are guided by that research. Many policies on parenting, for example, are inspired by research that finds a correlation between the behavior of parents and the behavior of children. Loving parents have confident children, authoritative parents (neither too permissive nor too punitive) have well-behaved children, parents who talk to their children have children with better language skills, and so on. Everyone concludes that to grow the best children, parents must be loving, authoritative, and talkative, and if children don't turn out well it must be the parents' fault. But the conclusions depend on the belief that children are blank slates. Parents, remember, provide their children with genes, not just a home environment. The correlations between parents and children may be telling us only that the same genes that make adults loving, authoritative, and talkative make their children self-confident, well- behaved, and articulate. Until the studies are redone with adopted children (who get only their environment, not their genes, from their parents), the data are compatible with the possibility that genes make all the difference, the possibility that parenting makes all the difference, or anything in between. Yet in almost every instance, the most extreme position -- that parents are everything -- is the only one researchers entertain. {x}
The taboo on human nature has not just put blinkers on researchers but turned any discussion of it into a heresy that must be stamped out. Many writers are so desperate to discredit any suggestion of an innate human constitution that they have thrown logic and civility out the window. Elementary distinctions -- "some" versus "all," "probable" versus "always," "is" versus "ought" -- are eagerly flouted to paint human nature as an extremist doctrine and thereby steer readers away from it. The analysis of ideas is commonly replaced by political smears and personal
? ? ? ? ? attacks. This poisoning of the intellectual atmosphere has left us unequipped to analyze pressing issues about human nature just as new scientific discoveries are making them acute.
The denial of human nature has spread beyond the academy and has led to a disconnect between intellectual life and common sense. I first had the idea of writing this book when I started a collection of astonishing claims from pundits and social critics about the malleability of the human psyche: that little boys quarrel and fight because they are encouraged to do so; that children enjoy sweets because their parents use them as a reward for eating vegetables; that teenagers get the idea to compete in looks and fashion from spelling bees and academic prizes; that men think the goal of sex is an orgasm because of the way they were socialized. The problem is not just that these claims are preposterous but that the writers did not acknowledge they were saying things that common sense might call into question. This is the mentality of a cult, in which fantastical beliefs are flaunted as proof of one's piety. That mentality cannot coexist with an esteem for the truth, and I believe it is responsible for some of the unfortunate trends in recent intellectual life. One trend is a stated contempt among many scholars for the concepts of truth, logic, and evidence. Another is a hypocritical divide between what intellectuals say in public and what they really believe. A third is the inevitable reaction: a culture of "politically incorrect" shock jocks who revel in anti-intellectualism and bigotry, emboldened by the knowledge that the intellectual establishment has forfeited claims to credibility in the eyes of the public.
Finally, the denial of human nature has not just corrupted the world of critics and intellectuals but has done harm to the lives of real people. The theory that parents can mold their children like clay has inflicted childrearing regimes on parents that are unnatural and sometimes cruel. It has distorted the choices faced by mothers as they try to balance their lives, and multiplied the anguish of parents whose children haven't turned out the way they hoped. The belief that human tastes are reversible cultural preferences has led social planners to write off people's enjoyment of ornament, natural light, and human scale and force millions of people to live in drab cement boxes. The romantic notion that all evil is a product of society has justified the release of dangerous psychopaths who promptly murdered innocent people. And the conviction {xi} that humanity could be reshaped by massive social engineering projects led to some of the greatest atrocities in history.
Though many of my arguments will be coolly analytical -- that an acknowledgment of human nature does not, logically speaking, imply the negative outcomes so many people fear -- I will not try to hide my belief that they have a positive thrust as well. "Man will become better when you show him what he is like," wrote Chekhov, and so the new sciences of human nature can help lead the way to a realistic, biologically informed humanism. They expose the psychological unity of our species beneath the superficial differences of physical appearance and parochial culture. They make us appreciate the wondrous complexity of the human mind, which we are apt to take for granted precisely because it works so well. They identify the moral intuitions that we can put to work in improving our lot. They promise a naturalness in human relationships, encouraging us to treat people in terms of how they do feel rather than how some theory says they ought to feel. They offer a touchstone by which we can identify suffering and oppression wherever they occur, unmasking the rationalizations of the powerful. They give us a way to see through the designs of self-appointed social reformers who would liberate us from our pleasures. They renew our appreciation for the achievements of democracy and of the rule of law. And they enhance the insights of artists and philosophers who have reflected on the human condition for millennia.
THE BLANK SLATE
Steven Pinker is Johnstone Professor of Psychology at Harvard University. His research on visual cognition and the psychology of language has earned prizes from the National Academy of Sciences and the American Psychological Association. Pinker has also received many awards for his teaching at MIT and for his books How the Mind Works (which was also a finalist for the Pulitzer Prize) and The Language Instinct. He is an elected fellow of several scientific societies, associate editor of Cognition, and a member of the usage panel of the American Heritage Dictionary. He has written for The New York Times, Time, The New Yorker, The New Republic, Slate, and Technology Review.
~
Praise for The Blank Slate
"A brilliant and forceful summary. . . A well-informed and well-written account of [human] limitations, [written with] a graceful interleaving of scientific and literary sources. . . . [This] fine book helps with a task that we all must begin to
? ? ? take seriously. . . Can it be that we have finally grown up? "
-- Melvin Konner, The American Prospect
"This is a brilliant book. It is beautifully written, and addresses profound issues with courage and clarity. There is nothing else like it, and it is going to have an impact that extends well beyond the scientific academy. "
-- Paul Bloom, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
"Steven Pinker has written an extremely good book -- clear, well argued, fair, learned, tough, witty, humane, stimulating. I only hope that people study it carefully before rising up ideologically against him. If they do, they will see that the idea of an innately flawed but wonderfully rich human nature is a force for good, not evil.
-- Colin McGinn, The Washington Post
"Steven Pinker is a man of encyclopedic knowledge and an incisive style of argument. His argument in The Blank Slate is that intellectual life in the West, and much of our social and political policy, was increasingly dominated through the twentieth century by a view of human nature that is fundamentally flawed; that this domination has been backed by something that amounts to academic terrorism (he does not put it quite so strongly): and that we would benefit {ii} substantially from a more realistic view. Pinker's exposition is thoroughly readable and of enviable clarity. His explanation of such a difficult technical matter as the analysis of variance and regression in twin studies, for example, would be very hard to better. He is not afraid of using strong language. . . in addition, parts of the book are delightfully funny. "
-- John R. G. Turner, The Times Literary Supplement
"Anyone who has read Pinker's earlier books -- including How the Mind Works and The Language Instinct -- will rightly guess that his latest effort is similarly sweeping, erudite, sharply argued, richly footnoted and fun to read. It's
? also highly persuasive. "
-- Michael Lemonick, Time
"[Pinker] makes his main argument persuasively and with great verve. . .
The Blank Slate ought to be read by anybody who feels they have had enough of nature-nurture rows or who thinks they already know where they stand on the science wars. It could change their minds. . . If nothing else, Mr. Pinker's book is a wonderfully readable taster of new research, much of it ingenious, designed to show that many more of our emotional biases and mental aptitudes than previously thought are hard-wired or, to use the old word, innate. . . This is a breath of air for a topic that has been politicized for too long. "
-- The Economist
"[Pinker] wades resolutely into the comforting gloom surrounding these not quite forbidden topics and calmly, lucidly marshals the facts to ground his strikingly subversive Darwinian claims -- subversive not of any of the things we properly hold dear but subversive of the phony protective layers of misinformation surrounding them. . . My reservations with Pinker's view [will be resolved] in the bright light of rational inquiry that he brings to these
important topics. "
-- Dan Dennett, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate brilliantly delineates the current state of play in the nature-nurture debate. Read it to understand not just the moral and aesthetic blindness of your friends, but the misguided idealism of nations. A magnificent and
timely work. "
-- Fay Weldon, The Daily Telegraph
"[Pinker] points us in the direction of a more productive debate, a debate in which the implications of science are confronted forthrightly and not simply wished away by politicized scientists. "
-- Francis Fukuyama, The Wall Street Journal {iii}
"The Blank Slate is. . . a stylish piece of work. I won't say it is better than The Language Instinct or How the Mind Works, but it is as good -- which is very high praise indeed. What a superb thinker and writer he is: what a role model to young scientists. And how courageous to buck the liberal trend in science, while remaining in person the best sort of liberal. Pinker is a star, and the world of science is lucky to have him. "
-- Richard Dawkins, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate is not dismal at all, but unexpectedly bracing. It feels a bit like being burgled. You're shocked, your things are gone, but you can't help thinking about how you're going to replace them. What Steven Pinker has done is
? break into our common human home and steal our illusions. "
-- John Morrish, The Independent
"As a brightly lighted path between what we would like to believe and what we need to know, [The Blank Slate] is required reading. Pinker presents an unanswerable case for accepting that man can be, as he is, both wired and free. "
-- Frederic Raphael, Los Angeles Times
"Pinker's thinking and writing are first-rate; maybe even better than that. The Blank Slate is much-needed, long overdue and -- if you are interested in what might be called the 'human nature wars' -- somewhere between that old standby, 'required reading,' and downright indispensable. It is unlikely to change the minds of those who are rigidly committed to the blank slate perspective, but for anyone whose 'nature' includes even a modicum of open- mindedness, it should prove a revelation. "
-- David Barash, Human Nature Review "Pinker is one of those rare writers who is at once persuasive and comprehensive, informative and entertaining. "
-- Kevin Shapiro, Commentary
"The fight for a separation of politics from science is an eminently sensible, logical, and ultimately humanistic task, and it took someone as brave as Pinker to dedicate himself to it. . . . [This is a] necessary book, a book that in a more truthful intellectual climate -- one open to the idea that any knowledge about ourselves can only enhance our ability to act well and compassionately -- would not have had to be written. In this climate, however, we should be grateful that it was. "
-- Daniel Smith, The Boston Globe {iv}
"The Blank Slate deserves to be read carefully and with an open mind. . . This landmark book makes an important contribution to the argument about nature vs. nurture in humans. Whether or not most readers end up on Pinker's side of the fence, one can hope that his thoroughness and reasoning will shed light into the darker corners where research has been suppressed by taboos, and where freedom of thought and speech have been inhibited by fear of consequences for asking forbidden questions. "
-- Nancy Jeannette Friedlander, The San Diego Union-Tribune
"This book is a modern magnum opus. The scholarship alone is mind-boggling, a monument of careful research, meticulous citation, breadth of input from diverse fields, great writing and humor. "
-- Tom Paskal, The Montreal Gazette "A delightfully provocative read. . . A constantly dynamic, if tacit, exchange between the author and his readers. "
-- Patrick Watson, The Globe and Mail
"A feast of a book. Pinker's analytical and impish mind ranges from Charles Darwin to Abigail Van Buren, from scientific studies to Annie Hall. . . . It will be a rare reader who agrees with everything in this book. But it is an
? intelligent book that says what it means and thinks about what it is saying. . . . Though much of the book is about human differences, the bigger idea is inherited similarity -- the 'psychological unity of our species. ' It is not a blank slate but a slate with a face -- a face that might be called human nature. When Pinker starts describing it, the reader will surely recognize it. "
{v}
THE BLANK SLATE
The Modern Denial of Human Nature
Steven Pinker
PENGUIN BOOKS
-- Bruce Ramsey, The Seattle Times
? ? ? ? ? ? <<
{vi} >> To Don, Judy, Leda, and John
PENGUIN BOOKS
Published by the Penguin Group
Penguin Group (USA) Inc. , 375 Hudson Street, New York, New York 10014, U. S. A.
Penguin Books Ltd, 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
Penguin Books Australia Ltd, 250 Camberwell Road, Camberwell, Victoria 3124, Australia Penguin Books Canada Ltd, 10 Alcorn Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M4V 3B2
Penguin Books India (P) Ltd, 11 Community Centre, Panchsheel Park, New Delhi - 110 017, India Penguin Books (N. Z. ) Ltd, Cnr Rosedale and Airborne Roads, Albany, Auckland, New Zealand Penguin Books (South Africa) (Pty) Ltd, 24 Sturdee Avenue, Rosebank, Johannesburg 2196, South Africa
Penguin Books Ltd, Registered Offices: 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
First published in the United States of America by Viking Penguin, a member of Penguin Putnam Inc. 2002
Published in Penguin Books 2003
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Copyright (C) Steven Pinker, 2002 All rights reserved
? ? ? ? Grateful acknowledgment is made for permission to reprint the following copyrighted material. Page 22: Lyrics from "A Simple Desultory Philippic (or How I Was Robert McNamara'd into Submission)"; copyright (C) 1965, Paul Simon; used by permission of the publisher: Paul Simon Music. Page 57: Chart, "Percentage of Male Deaths Caused by Warfare," from War Before Civilization by Lawrence H. Keeley, copyright (C) 1996 by Oxford University Press, Inc. ; used by permission of Oxford University Press, Inc. Page 88: Diagram of the wiring of the primate visual system from Michael Gazzaniga, The Cognitive Neurosciences, The MIT Press (1996). Page 179: Lyrics from "Gee, Officer Krupke" by Leonard Bernstein & Stephen Sondheim; (C) 1956, Amberson Holdings LLC and Stephen Sondheim; copyright renewed; Leonard Bernstein Music
Publishing Company LLC, publisher; used by permission. Page 199: Diagram, "Turning the Tables," from Mind Sights by Roger N. Shepard, (C) 1990 by Roger N. Shepard; reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and Company, LLC. "Checker Shadow Illusion" (C) Edward Adelson, 2002; reprinted with permission. Page 326: Lyrics from "You Don't Mess Around with Jim," written by Jim Croce; (C) 1972 (renewed), Time in a Bottle/Croce Publishing (ASCAP); all rights reserved; used by
permission.
THE LIBRARY OF CONGRESS HAS CATALOGUED THE HARDCOVER EDITION AS FOLLOWS: Pinker, Steven, 1954-
The blank slate : the modern denial of human nature / Steven Pinker.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and index.
ISBN 0-670-03151-8 (hc. )
ISBN 0 14 20. 0334 4 (pbk. )
1. Nature and nurture. I. Title.
BF341. P47 2002 155. 2'34 -- dc21 2002022719
Printed in the United States of America Set in Minion
Except in the United States of America, this book is sold subject to the condition that it shall not, by way of trade or otherwise, be lent, re-sold, hired out, or otherwise circulated without the publisher's prior consent in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition including this condition being imposed on the subsequent
purchaser.
The scanning, uploading and distribution of this book via the Internet or via any other means without the permission of the publisher is illegal and punishable by law. Please purchase only authorized electronic editions, and do not participate in or encourage electronic piracy of copyrighted materials. Your support of the author's rights is appreciated.
<< {vii} >> PREFACE
"Not another book on nature and nurture! Are there really people out there who still believe that the mind is a blank slate? Isn't it obvious to anyone with more than one child, to anyone who has been in a heterosexual relationship, or to anyone who has noticed that children learn language but house pets don't, that people are born with certain talents and temperaments? Haven't we all moved beyond the simplistic dichotomy between heredity and environment and realized that all behavior comes out of an interaction between the two? "
This is the kind of reaction I got from colleagues when I explained my plans for this book. At first glance the reaction is not unreasonable. Maybe nature versus nurture is a dead issue. Anyone familiar with current writings on mind and behavior has seen claims to the middle ground like these:
If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with this issue. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.
This is not going to be one of those books that says everything is genetic: it isn't. The environment is just as important as the genes. The things children experience while they are growing up are just as important as the things they are born with.
Even when a behavior is heritable, an individual's behavior is still a product of development, and thus it has a causal environmental component. . . . The modern understanding of how phenotypes are inherited through the replication of both genetic and environmental {viii} conditions suggests that. . . cultural traditions -- behaviors copied by children from their parents -- are likely to be crucial.
If you think these are innocuous compromises that show that everyone has outgrown the nature-nurture debate, think again. The quotations come, in fact, from three of the most incendiary books of the last decade. The first is from The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, who argue that the difference in average IQ scores between American blacks and American whites has both genetic and environmental causes. 1 The second is from The Nurture Assumption by Judith Rich Harris, who argues that children's personalities are shaped by their genes as well as by
? ? ? ? ? ? ? their environments, so similarities between children and their parents may come from their shared genes and not just from the effects of parenting. 2 The third is from A Natural History of Rape by Randy Thornhill and Craig Palmer,
who argue that rape is not simply a product of culture but also has roots in the nature of men's sexuality. 3 For invoking nurture and nature, not nurture alone, these authors have been picketed, shouted down, subjected to searing invective in the press, even denounced in Congress. Others expressing such opinions have been censored, assaulted, or threatened with criminal prosecution. 4
The idea that nature and nurture interact to shape some part of the mind might turn out to be wrong, but it is not wishy-washy or unexceptionable, even in the twenty-first century, thousands of years after the issue was framed. When it comes to explaining human thought and behavior, the possibility that heredity plays any role at all still has the power to shock. To acknowledge human nature, many think, is to endorse racism, sexism, war, greed, genocide, nihilism, reactionary politics, and neglect of children and the disadvantaged. Any claim that the mind has an innate organization strikes people not as a hypothesis that might be incorrect but as a thought it is immoral to think.
This book is about the moral, emotional, and political colorings of the concept of human nature in modern life. I will retrace the history that led people to see human nature as a dangerous idea, and I will try to unsnarl the moral and political rat's nests that have entangled the idea along the way. Though no book on human nature can hope to be uncontroversial, I did not write it to be yet another "explosive" book, as dust jackets tend to say.
A magnificent and
timely work. "
-- Fay Weldon, The Daily Telegraph
"[Pinker] points us in the direction of a more productive debate, a debate in which the implications of science are confronted forthrightly and not simply wished away by politicized scientists. "
-- Francis Fukuyama, The Wall Street Journal {iii}
"The Blank Slate is. . . a stylish piece of work. I won't say it is better than The Language Instinct or How the Mind Works, but it is as good -- which is very high praise indeed. What a superb thinker and writer he is: what a role model to young scientists. And how courageous to buck the liberal trend in science, while remaining in person the best sort of liberal. Pinker is a star, and the world of science is lucky to have him. "
-- Richard Dawkins, The Times Literary Supplement
"The Blank Slate is not dismal at all, but unexpectedly bracing. It feels a bit like being burgled. You're shocked, your things are gone, but you can't help thinking about how you're going to replace them. What Steven Pinker has done is
? break into our common human home and steal our illusions. "
-- John Morrish, The Independent
"As a brightly lighted path between what we would like to believe and what we need to know, [The Blank Slate] is required reading. Pinker presents an unanswerable case for accepting that man can be, as he is, both wired and free. "
-- Frederic Raphael, Los Angeles Times
"Pinker's thinking and writing are first-rate; maybe even better than that. The Blank Slate is much-needed, long overdue and -- if you are interested in what might be called the 'human nature wars' -- somewhere between that old standby, 'required reading,' and downright indispensable. It is unlikely to change the minds of those who are rigidly committed to the blank slate perspective, but for anyone whose 'nature' includes even a modicum of open- mindedness, it should prove a revelation. "
-- David Barash, Human Nature Review "Pinker is one of those rare writers who is at once persuasive and comprehensive, informative and entertaining. "
-- Kevin Shapiro, Commentary
"The fight for a separation of politics from science is an eminently sensible, logical, and ultimately humanistic task, and it took someone as brave as Pinker to dedicate himself to it. . . . [This is a] necessary book, a book that in a more truthful intellectual climate -- one open to the idea that any knowledge about ourselves can only enhance our ability to act well and compassionately -- would not have had to be written. In this climate, however, we should be grateful that it was. "
-- Daniel Smith, The Boston Globe {iv}
"The Blank Slate deserves to be read carefully and with an open mind. . . This landmark book makes an important contribution to the argument about nature vs. nurture in humans. Whether or not most readers end up on Pinker's side of the fence, one can hope that his thoroughness and reasoning will shed light into the darker corners where research has been suppressed by taboos, and where freedom of thought and speech have been inhibited by fear of consequences for asking forbidden questions. "
-- Nancy Jeannette Friedlander, The San Diego Union-Tribune
"This book is a modern magnum opus. The scholarship alone is mind-boggling, a monument of careful research, meticulous citation, breadth of input from diverse fields, great writing and humor. "
-- Tom Paskal, The Montreal Gazette "A delightfully provocative read. . . A constantly dynamic, if tacit, exchange between the author and his readers. "
-- Patrick Watson, The Globe and Mail
"A feast of a book. Pinker's analytical and impish mind ranges from Charles Darwin to Abigail Van Buren, from scientific studies to Annie Hall. . . . It will be a rare reader who agrees with everything in this book. But it is an
? intelligent book that says what it means and thinks about what it is saying. . . . Though much of the book is about human differences, the bigger idea is inherited similarity -- the 'psychological unity of our species. ' It is not a blank slate but a slate with a face -- a face that might be called human nature. When Pinker starts describing it, the reader will surely recognize it. "
{v}
THE BLANK SLATE
The Modern Denial of Human Nature
Steven Pinker
PENGUIN BOOKS
-- Bruce Ramsey, The Seattle Times
? ? ? ? ? ? <<
{vi} >> To Don, Judy, Leda, and John
PENGUIN BOOKS
Published by the Penguin Group
Penguin Group (USA) Inc. , 375 Hudson Street, New York, New York 10014, U. S. A.
Penguin Books Ltd, 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
Penguin Books Australia Ltd, 250 Camberwell Road, Camberwell, Victoria 3124, Australia Penguin Books Canada Ltd, 10 Alcorn Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M4V 3B2
Penguin Books India (P) Ltd, 11 Community Centre, Panchsheel Park, New Delhi - 110 017, India Penguin Books (N. Z. ) Ltd, Cnr Rosedale and Airborne Roads, Albany, Auckland, New Zealand Penguin Books (South Africa) (Pty) Ltd, 24 Sturdee Avenue, Rosebank, Johannesburg 2196, South Africa
Penguin Books Ltd, Registered Offices: 80 Strand, London WC2R ORL, England
First published in the United States of America by Viking Penguin, a member of Penguin Putnam Inc. 2002
Published in Penguin Books 2003
1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Copyright (C) Steven Pinker, 2002 All rights reserved
? ? ? ? Grateful acknowledgment is made for permission to reprint the following copyrighted material. Page 22: Lyrics from "A Simple Desultory Philippic (or How I Was Robert McNamara'd into Submission)"; copyright (C) 1965, Paul Simon; used by permission of the publisher: Paul Simon Music. Page 57: Chart, "Percentage of Male Deaths Caused by Warfare," from War Before Civilization by Lawrence H. Keeley, copyright (C) 1996 by Oxford University Press, Inc. ; used by permission of Oxford University Press, Inc. Page 88: Diagram of the wiring of the primate visual system from Michael Gazzaniga, The Cognitive Neurosciences, The MIT Press (1996). Page 179: Lyrics from "Gee, Officer Krupke" by Leonard Bernstein & Stephen Sondheim; (C) 1956, Amberson Holdings LLC and Stephen Sondheim; copyright renewed; Leonard Bernstein Music
Publishing Company LLC, publisher; used by permission. Page 199: Diagram, "Turning the Tables," from Mind Sights by Roger N. Shepard, (C) 1990 by Roger N. Shepard; reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and Company, LLC. "Checker Shadow Illusion" (C) Edward Adelson, 2002; reprinted with permission. Page 326: Lyrics from "You Don't Mess Around with Jim," written by Jim Croce; (C) 1972 (renewed), Time in a Bottle/Croce Publishing (ASCAP); all rights reserved; used by
permission.
THE LIBRARY OF CONGRESS HAS CATALOGUED THE HARDCOVER EDITION AS FOLLOWS: Pinker, Steven, 1954-
The blank slate : the modern denial of human nature / Steven Pinker.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and index.
ISBN 0-670-03151-8 (hc. )
ISBN 0 14 20. 0334 4 (pbk. )
1. Nature and nurture. I. Title.
BF341. P47 2002 155. 2'34 -- dc21 2002022719
Printed in the United States of America Set in Minion
Except in the United States of America, this book is sold subject to the condition that it shall not, by way of trade or otherwise, be lent, re-sold, hired out, or otherwise circulated without the publisher's prior consent in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition including this condition being imposed on the subsequent
purchaser.
The scanning, uploading and distribution of this book via the Internet or via any other means without the permission of the publisher is illegal and punishable by law. Please purchase only authorized electronic editions, and do not participate in or encourage electronic piracy of copyrighted materials. Your support of the author's rights is appreciated.
<< {vii} >> PREFACE
"Not another book on nature and nurture! Are there really people out there who still believe that the mind is a blank slate? Isn't it obvious to anyone with more than one child, to anyone who has been in a heterosexual relationship, or to anyone who has noticed that children learn language but house pets don't, that people are born with certain talents and temperaments? Haven't we all moved beyond the simplistic dichotomy between heredity and environment and realized that all behavior comes out of an interaction between the two? "
This is the kind of reaction I got from colleagues when I explained my plans for this book. At first glance the reaction is not unreasonable. Maybe nature versus nurture is a dead issue. Anyone familiar with current writings on mind and behavior has seen claims to the middle ground like these:
If the reader is now convinced that either the genetic or environmental explanation has won out to the exclusion of the other, we have not done a sufficiently good job of presenting one side or the other. It seems highly likely to us that both genes and environment have something to do with this issue. What might the mix be? We are resolutely agnostic on that issue; as far as we can determine, the evidence does not yet justify an estimate.
This is not going to be one of those books that says everything is genetic: it isn't. The environment is just as important as the genes. The things children experience while they are growing up are just as important as the things they are born with.
Even when a behavior is heritable, an individual's behavior is still a product of development, and thus it has a causal environmental component. . . . The modern understanding of how phenotypes are inherited through the replication of both genetic and environmental {viii} conditions suggests that. . . cultural traditions -- behaviors copied by children from their parents -- are likely to be crucial.
If you think these are innocuous compromises that show that everyone has outgrown the nature-nurture debate, think again. The quotations come, in fact, from three of the most incendiary books of the last decade. The first is from The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, who argue that the difference in average IQ scores between American blacks and American whites has both genetic and environmental causes. 1 The second is from The Nurture Assumption by Judith Rich Harris, who argues that children's personalities are shaped by their genes as well as by
? ? ? ? ? ? ? their environments, so similarities between children and their parents may come from their shared genes and not just from the effects of parenting. 2 The third is from A Natural History of Rape by Randy Thornhill and Craig Palmer,
who argue that rape is not simply a product of culture but also has roots in the nature of men's sexuality. 3 For invoking nurture and nature, not nurture alone, these authors have been picketed, shouted down, subjected to searing invective in the press, even denounced in Congress. Others expressing such opinions have been censored, assaulted, or threatened with criminal prosecution. 4
The idea that nature and nurture interact to shape some part of the mind might turn out to be wrong, but it is not wishy-washy or unexceptionable, even in the twenty-first century, thousands of years after the issue was framed. When it comes to explaining human thought and behavior, the possibility that heredity plays any role at all still has the power to shock. To acknowledge human nature, many think, is to endorse racism, sexism, war, greed, genocide, nihilism, reactionary politics, and neglect of children and the disadvantaged. Any claim that the mind has an innate organization strikes people not as a hypothesis that might be incorrect but as a thought it is immoral to think.
This book is about the moral, emotional, and political colorings of the concept of human nature in modern life. I will retrace the history that led people to see human nature as a dangerous idea, and I will try to unsnarl the moral and political rat's nests that have entangled the idea along the way. Though no book on human nature can hope to be uncontroversial, I did not write it to be yet another "explosive" book, as dust jackets tend to say. I am not, as many people assume, countering an extreme "nurture" position with an extreme "nature" position, with the truth lying somewhere in between. In some cases, an extreme environmentalist explanation is correct: which language you speak is an obvious example, and differences among races and ethnic groups in test scores may be another. In other cases, such as certain inherited neurological disorders, an extreme hereditarian explanation is correct. In most cases the correct explanation will invoke a complex interaction between heredity and environment: culture is crucial, but culture could not exist without mental {ix} faculties that allow humans to create and learn culture to begin with. My goal in this book is not to argue that genes are everything and culture is nothing -- no one believes that -- but to explore why the extreme position (that culture is everything) is so often seen as moderate, and the moderate position is seen as extreme.
Nor does acknowledging human nature have the political implications so many fear. It does not, for example, require one to abandon feminism, or to accept current levels of inequality or violence, or to treat morality as a fiction. For the most part I will try not to advocate particular policies or to advance the agenda of the political left or right. I believe that controversies about policy almost always involve tradeoffs between competing values, and that science is equipped to identify the tradeoffs but not to resolve them. Many of these tradeoffs, I will show, arise from features of human nature, and by clarifying them I hope to make our collective choices, whatever they are, better informed. If I am an advocate, it is for discoveries about human nature that have been ignored or suppressed in modern discussions of human affairs.
Why is it important to sort this all out? The refusal to acknowledge human nature is like the Victorians' embarrassment about sex, only worse: it distorts our science and scholarship, our public discourse, and our day-to- day lives. Logicians tell us that a single contradiction can corrupt a set of statements and allow falsehoods to proliferate through it. The dogma that human nature does not exist, in the face of evidence from science and common sense that it does, is just such a corrupting influence.
First, the doctrine that the mind is a blank slate has distorted the study of human beings, and thus the public and private decisions that are guided by that research. Many policies on parenting, for example, are inspired by research that finds a correlation between the behavior of parents and the behavior of children. Loving parents have confident children, authoritative parents (neither too permissive nor too punitive) have well-behaved children, parents who talk to their children have children with better language skills, and so on. Everyone concludes that to grow the best children, parents must be loving, authoritative, and talkative, and if children don't turn out well it must be the parents' fault. But the conclusions depend on the belief that children are blank slates. Parents, remember, provide their children with genes, not just a home environment. The correlations between parents and children may be telling us only that the same genes that make adults loving, authoritative, and talkative make their children self-confident, well- behaved, and articulate. Until the studies are redone with adopted children (who get only their environment, not their genes, from their parents), the data are compatible with the possibility that genes make all the difference, the possibility that parenting makes all the difference, or anything in between. Yet in almost every instance, the most extreme position -- that parents are everything -- is the only one researchers entertain. {x}
The taboo on human nature has not just put blinkers on researchers but turned any discussion of it into a heresy that must be stamped out. Many writers are so desperate to discredit any suggestion of an innate human constitution that they have thrown logic and civility out the window. Elementary distinctions -- "some" versus "all," "probable" versus "always," "is" versus "ought" -- are eagerly flouted to paint human nature as an extremist doctrine and thereby steer readers away from it. The analysis of ideas is commonly replaced by political smears and personal
? ? ? ? ? attacks. This poisoning of the intellectual atmosphere has left us unequipped to analyze pressing issues about human nature just as new scientific discoveries are making them acute.
The denial of human nature has spread beyond the academy and has led to a disconnect between intellectual life and common sense. I first had the idea of writing this book when I started a collection of astonishing claims from pundits and social critics about the malleability of the human psyche: that little boys quarrel and fight because they are encouraged to do so; that children enjoy sweets because their parents use them as a reward for eating vegetables; that teenagers get the idea to compete in looks and fashion from spelling bees and academic prizes; that men think the goal of sex is an orgasm because of the way they were socialized. The problem is not just that these claims are preposterous but that the writers did not acknowledge they were saying things that common sense might call into question. This is the mentality of a cult, in which fantastical beliefs are flaunted as proof of one's piety. That mentality cannot coexist with an esteem for the truth, and I believe it is responsible for some of the unfortunate trends in recent intellectual life. One trend is a stated contempt among many scholars for the concepts of truth, logic, and evidence. Another is a hypocritical divide between what intellectuals say in public and what they really believe. A third is the inevitable reaction: a culture of "politically incorrect" shock jocks who revel in anti-intellectualism and bigotry, emboldened by the knowledge that the intellectual establishment has forfeited claims to credibility in the eyes of the public.
Finally, the denial of human nature has not just corrupted the world of critics and intellectuals but has done harm to the lives of real people. The theory that parents can mold their children like clay has inflicted childrearing regimes on parents that are unnatural and sometimes cruel. It has distorted the choices faced by mothers as they try to balance their lives, and multiplied the anguish of parents whose children haven't turned out the way they hoped. The belief that human tastes are reversible cultural preferences has led social planners to write off people's enjoyment of ornament, natural light, and human scale and force millions of people to live in drab cement boxes. The romantic notion that all evil is a product of society has justified the release of dangerous psychopaths who promptly murdered innocent people. And the conviction {xi} that humanity could be reshaped by massive social engineering projects led to some of the greatest atrocities in history.
Though many of my arguments will be coolly analytical -- that an acknowledgment of human nature does not, logically speaking, imply the negative outcomes so many people fear -- I will not try to hide my belief that they have a positive thrust as well. "Man will become better when you show him what he is like," wrote Chekhov, and so the new sciences of human nature can help lead the way to a realistic, biologically informed humanism. They expose the psychological unity of our species beneath the superficial differences of physical appearance and parochial culture. They make us appreciate the wondrous complexity of the human mind, which we are apt to take for granted precisely because it works so well. They identify the moral intuitions that we can put to work in improving our lot. They promise a naturalness in human relationships, encouraging us to treat people in terms of how they do feel rather than how some theory says they ought to feel. They offer a touchstone by which we can identify suffering and oppression wherever they occur, unmasking the rationalizations of the powerful. They give us a way to see through the designs of self-appointed social reformers who would liberate us from our pleasures. They renew our appreciation for the achievements of democracy and of the rule of law. And they enhance the insights of artists and philosophers who have reflected on the human condition for millennia.
